Deriving Appearance Scales
نویسندگان
چکیده
The concept of color space has come to be an unquestioned threedimensional representation of color stimuli, or color appearance, intended to simplify the relationships among physically measurable attributes of light, mathematical formulae, and human sensations and perceptions. The notion of three-dimensional mathematical spaces as adjuncts for color is often helpful, but perhaps also misleading at times. Color appearance models requiring five or six dimensions to represent color appearance illustrate some of the limitations of historic spaces. This paper poses the question of whether color appearance would be better represented by independent appearance scales with no requirement that they be related as a higher-dimensional space. In other words, is color better represented by six one-dimensional color scales than one or two three-dimensional color spaces. A framework for implementing such appearance scales is described and one implementation is presented along with discussion of the ramifications for color difference metrics. Introduction Color scientists and engineers have become accustomed to the fundamental concept of color space to the point that the concept itself goes unquestioned. Much like most accept the fact that the earth is nearly spherical, those in the color-related fields proceed merrily along without a doubt that color space is three dimensional. Further, some continue to seek the holy grail of a three-dimensional color space in which perceived color differences can be expressed as uniform Euclidean distances despite an apparent lack of psychophysical evidence that such a space might exist. Perhaps it is time to, once again, step back and ask the question of whether the concept of a Euclidean distance metric in three dimensions really makes sense for describing color, even approximately. Perhaps some insight into appropriate descriptions of color appearance can be gained from a cursory examination of the other human senses.[1,2] Our perception of taste has at least five distinct dimensions, sweetness, bitterness, sourness, saltiness, and umami, and seldom does anyone speak of changes in taste perceptions as a Euclidean difference space. Similarly our sense of smell is served by something on the order of 1000 different receptor types. Some have tried to reduce the dimensionality to approximately six including flowery, foul, fruity, spicy, burnt, and resinous. Our sense of hearing is actually spectral (plus intensity) in terms familiar to color scientists as humans are able to detect frequencies within sounds (no aural metamerism) and the relative intensities of each frequency. Finally our sense of touch might well be too complex to even attempt to summarize in a sentence or two. None of the perceptions arising from any of these senses are commonly expressed in terms of multi-dimensional spaces with Euclidean (or similar) difference metrics. Given these similarities in our other senses, why should we think color is different? Is it the relatively low dimensionality? Is it the seemingly simple perceptual relationships such as color opponency? Is it the nature of additive color mixing? Additive color mixture under photopic conditions provides ample evidence for trichromacy, the three-dimensional nature of color matching/mixture. Adding Grassmann’s laws allows expression of color matches in various sets of primaries via simple 3x3 linear transformations analogous to a change of basis in a three-dimensional, linear space (where Euclidean distances mean something mathematically). Perhaps it is this property of color matching, which is not a direct representation of perception or appearance, that leads to an almost irresistible next step to start expressing color matches in three-dimensional Euclidean spaces. And then, apparently without clear justification, the concept is carried forward in attempts to express appearances and differences in similar three-dimensional Euclidean spaces such as the CIELAB color space. Perhaps those attempts were always as doomed as any explorers who might have set out to “circumnavigate” a flat earth. Color science is not devoid of examples typically described as color spaces that are actually descriptions of color perception one dimension at a time.[3] For example, the Munsell system, despite its common embodiments, was derived as a system of three independent perceptual dimensions, hue, value, and chroma. Similarly, Guth’s ATD model of visual perception was typically described in terms of independent dimensions, although the temptation to plot some of them together for some examples proved irresistible. Likewise, color appearance models such as CIECAM02 were developed with independent predictors of the six perceptual dimensions of brightness, lightness, colorfulness, saturation, chroma, and hue. This was somewhat compromised by requests for rectangular color space dimensions which appeared as CIECAM97s evolved to CIECAM02. However it should be noted that cylindrical representations of the appearance spaces were common even before the requests for rectangular coordinates. Lastly, the NCS system provides a useful example of hue being treated separately from whiteness-blackness and chromaticness. And while NCS whiteness-blackness and chromaticness are plotted in two-dimensional trilinear form, the dimensions are largely independent since the anchor of maximal chromaticness appropriately varies from hue to hue. All of this insight leads to the hypothesis that perhaps color space is actually a one-dimensional space, rather than a threedimensional space, and that Euclidean distance metrics might indeed be successful in such a space. Of course, color appearance cannot be properly described in a single one-dimensional space. Instead six of them are required. There are three fundamental appearance attributes for related colors, lightness, saturation, and hue. Combined with information about absolute luminance, colorfulness and brightness can be derived from these and are important and useful appearance attributes. Lastly, chroma can be derived from lightness and saturation if desired as an alternative relative colorfulness metric. Thus, color is rightfully and fully described with six one-dimensional appearance spaces (or scales), four of which are fundamental for related colors and two of which are derived from the fundamental scales. This paper provides some detail of the conceptual framework of a color model made up of one-dimensional spaces and an implementation of that framework for future application and investigation. Note: One-dimensional “spaces” are more commonly referred to as “scales” in color science, thus the term “scale” is used preferentially for the remainder of the paper. Conceptual Framework A set of color appearance scales (or dimensions, or spaces) following these principles has been derived and an implementation is presented in the next section. This section provides the general framework that could be easily adapted to different specific implementations of the concept. The first step is to apply a chromatic adaptation model to compute corresponding colors for reference viewing conditions (e.g. D65, 315 cd/m2). Then the IPT model, derived specifically for accurate hue representations, is used to compute a hue angle (h) and then a hue composition (H) can be computed based on NCS unique hues. For the defined hue, saturation (S) is computed using the classical formula for excitation purity applied along lines of constant h in the u’v’ chromaticity diagram. For that chromaticity, the luminance for zero gray content, G0, is defined as the reference for lightness (L) computations that follow a power function with offset model found to perform well in recent research for high-dynamic-range lightness-brightness scaling. The remaining dimensions are then derived from L and S along with luminance information. Brightness (B) is lightness (L) scaled by a factor derived from the classic work of Stevens and Stevens that illustrated terminal brightness as a function of adapting luminance. The derived scales are colorfulness (C), which is simply saturation (S) scaled by brightness (B), and chroma (Ch) which is saturation (S) times lightness (L). This type of formulation allows accurate description of color appearance for lights and objects across a variety of adaptation conditions and for lowor high-dynamic-range scenes. To the degree that each perceptual scale is accurate, differences on each of the dimensions should be easily calculated and, as long as the temptation to combine those differences into a single Euclidean distance metric is resisted, quite effective results can be obtained. The next section steps through a proposed implementation in detail. Implementation Fairchild,[4] at ISCC/IS&T/SID meeting on color spaces, outlined a methodology for computing the set of three fundamental appearance attributes of hue, saturation, and lightness for related colors from which the attributes of brightness and colorfulness can be derived as a function of the absolute luminance along with chroma. As the hue-linearized space IPT, based in opponent color theory, is considered exceptionally uniform in hue, the hue scale (h) is computed as a simple hue angle using the IPT model.[5] The required inputs for the IPT hue angle computation are the CIE tristimulus values in XYZ for the corresponding colors in CIE Illuminant D65. A chromatic adaptation transform is required to obtain corresponding colors for Illuminant D65 if the stimuli of interest are viewed under a different state of adaptation. The CAT02 transformation imbedded in the CIECAM02 color appearance model is recommended with a simple von Kries transformation on cone fundamentals a second choice. If luminance information is available and impacted by the selected chromatic adaptation transformation, then transformation to a white-point luminance of cd/m2 is recommended. Hue composition (H) can be obtained by recognizing that the NCS unique hues fall, on average at hue angles of 27.4, 89.8, 162.2, 231.3 degrees for red, yellow, green, and blue respectively. Hue composition is computed simply as percentages between these four anchor hues as done in other color appearance models. Figure 1. NCS unique hues plotted in IPT to illustrate the definition of hue (h) and hue composition (H). Saturation (S) is computed in the classical way from excitation purity at the computed IPT hue in the u’v’ chromaticity diagram. A slight modification is made in the distances used in the computation however. Saturation is computed as the ratio of the distance from the white point (D65) to the stimulus in question to the distance from the white point (D65) to the spectrum locus stimulus with the same hue angle (h) as the stimulus in question. In cases where IPT constant hue predictions fall on straight lines in the u’v’ diagram, this computation is identical with the traditional excitation purity computation. In cases where constant hue contours would be curved in u’v’ there are small differences in the calculation. This is illustrated in Fig. 2 and Eq. 1. Maximum color saturation occurs at the locus of pure color computed in a u’v’ chromaticity diagram by cascading the spectra of CIE Illuminant D65 with the 2o Observer and each of the monochromatic stimuli at every integral nanometer from 300 to 830 nanometers of the visible spectrum. A lookup table (LUT) in u’v’ as a function of IPT hue was then constructed in one degree increments from the corresponding IPT hue values computed on the locus of pure color. Figure 2 illustrates samples of the resulting locus of pure color in a u’v’ chromaticity diagram annotated with IPT hue angle. Saturation S was then computed according to Eq. 1. Figure 2. Saturation in a u’v’ chromaticity diagram annotated with IPT hue of a random sample from each of the twenty-four (24) NCS Aim Hues. S = ′ u − ′ u n ( ) + ′ v − ′ v n ( ) ′ u L − ′ u n ( ) + ′ v L − ′ v n ( ) (1) In this computation, u’v’ are the chromaticities of the stimulus, un’vn’ those of D65 diffuse white, and uL’vL’ at the spectrum locus for the same hue angle from the LUT of the chromaticities of pure color in IPT hue. Figure 2 illustrates both the sample color randomly sampled from each of the twenty-four Natural Color System aim hues and the pure color at that same IPT hue. For each hue, vectors are shown as a solid line for the spectrum locus and as dotted lines for the sample color. Lightness is computed from luminance for each color relative to the corresponding luminance at Evan’s G0 [6] from the relationships given by Nayatani [7]. G0 defines the luminance for each saturation at which stimuli of higher luminance appear self luminous and stimuli of lower luminance appear to have gray content (or appear like object colors). Thus the G0 luminance provides an appropriate reference for perceived lightness of object colors that accounts for the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch effect and discrepancies between photopic luminance predictions and heterochromatic brightness matches. Recently, Chen et al.[8] showed that lightness could be scaled both above and below diffuse white and that the perceptual results were well predicted by a lightness function of the form given in Eq. 2. Equation 2 defines the lightness scale as a power function with an offset term and its general form is illustrated in Fig. 3. L = 0.98Yg 1 2.3 + 0.02 (2) In Eq. 2, Yg is the luminance of the color relative to the luminance YG0 at G0 and its chromaticity. YG0 is given by finding relative luminance of the NCS color with minimum NCS blackness and the same chromaticities as the sample in question. The minimum value of NCS blackness is zero where grayness is said to be at G0. Figure 3. Log Lightness (L) as a function of sample log luminance relative to G0 luminance for the same hue (h) and saturation (S) This was accomplished for the value of the NCS blackness (swartz) derived from the chromaticities u’v’ of the sample color, according to a method given by Heckaman and Fairchild [9] with basis in Nayatani [7] and the regression technique prescribed by Derrefeldt and Sahlin [10] from conversion data between NCS units of blackness and chromaticness and CIE tristimulus values taken from Bencuya [11] as illustrated in Fig. 4. Figure 5 illustrates the “tent” of the relative luminance of G0 over the u’v’ chromaticity diagram. Figure 4. NCS blackness S as a function of CIELAB Lightness and Chroma for the NCS Aim hue B30G. Blue circles are computed from Bencuya [8] measured tristimulus values and the red crosses regressed according to Derrefeldt and Sahlin [7]. Brightness (Q) is computed as a scaled version of lightness (L). The scalar depends on the absolute luminance level and light adaptation to that level. This relationship has been described nicely by both Stevens and Stevens [12] and Evans [13] based on Marshall and Talbot.[14] In this implementation the terminal brightness locus of Stevens and Stevens is used to define the brightness scalar. (An alternative would be to base it on the FL function in CIECAM02 which serves a similar purpose.) The terminal brightness locus is the perceived brightness of a stimulus Figure 5. The “tent” of the relative luminance of in a u’v’ chromaticity
منابع مشابه
Dilational Hilbert Scales and Deconvolutional Sharpening
Operationally, index functions of variable Hilbert scales can be viewed as generators for families of spaces and norms. Using a one parameter family of index functions based on the dilations of a given index function, a new class of scales (dilational Hilbert scales (DHS)) is derived which generates new interpolatory inequalities (dilational interpolatory inequalities (DII)) which have the ordi...
متن کاملUpward and downward physical appearance comparisons: development of scales and examination of predictive qualities.
Despite good theoretical and empirical rationale for assessing tendencies to make upward and downward physical appearance comparisons no measure for these specific constructs exists. The present work developed and tested the psychometric properties of upward and downward physical appearance comparison scales. The scales were administered to participants (N=224) along with measures of general ap...
متن کاملThe Comparison of Body Image Between Athletes and Non-Athletes Postmenopausal Females
Objectives: The aim of the present study was to compare and contrastthe sub-scale of the body image of the athletes and nonathlete menopaused females. Methods & Materials: The subjects of the research comprise 60 individuals who were between 43-53 years old. In this study the personalinformation questionnaire and PSDQ test were used.The Personal information questionnaire included the sub-sca...
متن کاملGenealogical method of urban typo-morphology with the aim of deriving pattern for providing form-based codes
Introduction: The emergence of form-based codes (FBCs), along with the familiar and near-universal rejection of conventional zoning, is a complex story, and more interesting than might first be supposed. The Codes Study generally does not track developer-driven form-based codes. The socio-economic context of form-based codes has shown positive FBC impacts on physical and environmental well-bein...
متن کاملThe Moderated Relationship of Appearance Valence on Appearance Self Consciousness: Development and Testing of New Measures of Appearance Schema Components
This paper describes the creation and psychometric properties of two independent measures of aspects of appearance schematicity--appearance salience and valence, assessed by the CARSAL and CARVAL, and their relation to appearance self-consciousness. Five hundred and ninety two participants provided data in a web based task. The results demonstrate the sound psychometric properties of both scale...
متن کاملDilational interpolatory inequalities
Operationally, index functions of variable Hilbert scales can be viewed as generators for families of spaces and norms and, thereby, associated scales of interpolatory inequalities. Using one parameter families of index functions based on the dilations of given index functions, new classes of interpolatory inequalities, dilational interpolatory inequalities (DII), are constructed. They have ord...
متن کامل